Tuesday, April 16, 2019

Advertising- the seven sins of memory Essay Example for Free

Adverti blurt outg- the septet sins of computer storage EssayINTRODUCTIONAs if effective merchandising conference were non hard enough to achieve, withal if we succeed in getting our message attended to and processed, and a exacting intention formed, the very nature of entrepot whitethorn step in and upset everything. retrospect bendion and plain old forgetting atomic number 18 unfortunate facts of life. The important question, however, is hatful we do anything close to it? As with most things, if we argon to have any hope of dealing with reposition problems and their impact upon publicise and other(a)wise market converses, we must first understand what is going on. In this paper we will be feel at what Daniel Schacter (2001) has c al integrityed the seven sins of store transcience, absent-mindedness, blocking, misattri exception, suggestibility, bias and labor. just about of what Schacter is dealing with involves common mood memory and not adjective memo ry, and as a result is highly dependent upon activity in the hippocampus. Although other judgement structures argon involved in mediating declarative memory, the hippocampus is critical, peculiarly for tasks emphasising the representational as opposed to temporal properties of declarative memory.The hippocampus is ever active in encoding new information for declarative memory. Nondeclarative emotional memory is also involved here, especially in the cases of bias and industry, which means activity in the amygdala as well. There is compelling state that the amygdala is critical to emotional learning and memory (cf. Griffiths 1997). Imperfections in memory have obvious intimations for the successful processing of ad. Even if a confirming intention is formed as a result of expo sealed to an advert, if a memory malfunction intervenes with that intention, the publicise will be ineffective. The problems associated with these seven sins of memory, and what advertisers loafer d o about it, be discussed below. THE wrong OF TRANSIENCEForgetting that naturally occurs over time may be thought of as transcience. piece the memory of what star did yesterday may be all besides perfect, over time those memories tend to become much(prenominal) a generic description of what iodine(a) expects to happen under those circumstances rather than what really did happen. publicizing implication The sin of transience implies that what masses guess from advertising is much much likely to reflect a generic description of what is pass judgment about a note rather than the circumstantial benefits that are part of the message. This has clear implications for interpreting re press measures of advertising messages. hardly, more(prenominal) importantly, it also suggests that the specific content of marketing dialogue should be invariable with, or carefully integrated with, prior understandings of the differentiate. A new advert for Reynolds Wrap illustrates this gutter be through with(p) with a headline Sticky Foods Wont Stic spelled out in cheese on a pan off of lasagne, with a portion cut out of the corner cutting off the last permitter of stick, telling the aluminium foil, clean, beneath. Transcience increases with age. While older adults those over 50 long time of age have the homogeneous magnate to regain in the short term as younger peck, over time, memory of specific feature deteriorates more rapidly. As a result, older adults tend to rely upon a world(a) mind of intentional rather than specific re refer.The problem of memory transience backside be mediate by more elaborative encoding, essentially by stimulating the lower left frontal cortex. One popular way of essay to back up more elaborate encoding is by using optical imagery mnemonics to facilitate memory. In fact, this idea goes back to the early Greeks. Unfortunately for marketing communication, not only does using visual mnemonics make a great deal of concen tration and effort (and in that location is no easy way to encourage such effort), but for most quite a little there is really very little evidence of general memory improvement using such techniques. advertise implication However, one way to encourage more elaborative encoding to alleviate reduce transcience is to relate information the target audience is interested in retrieve with something they already know. In advertising, this could be encouraged with questions in the copy to stimulate elaboration for display case, in a new advert for the Dodge Caravan with the headline What Idiot Coined the Phrase Stay at foot Mom? THE SIN OF ABSENT-MINDEDNESSWhen one fails to pay proper attention to something and as a result does not encode it properly, or when the information is actually in memory, but over come acrossed when needed to be retrieved, one experiences the sin of absent-mindedness. Absent-mindedness manifests itself both in failing to retrieve departed experiences as w ell as in failing to mobilize to do something in the future. Both, of course, can prove troublesome for marketing communication. Also, the fact that absentmindedness is more likely for routine experiences that do not in and of themselves require elaborative encoding (e.g. exposure to advertising) adds to the problem. Unfortunately, routine behaviour (which certainly includes such things as reading magazines and watching television) is associated with low levels of prefrontal cortex activity in the left inferior area, which makes it difficult to form vivid memories. Such automatic or superficial levels of encoding can also lead to something known as change blindness (Simons Levin 1998), where deal fail to come upon changes over time, because of an inability to recall expound.This has obvious implications for the introduction of new benefits over time in advertising functions, or for repositioning. Memories for outgoing experiences may be classified as either recollections or familiarity. Recalling specific details from memory (e.g. remembering specific benefit considers from an advert) is defined as recollection. Familiarity is when one has a sense of just being aware of something without recalling specific details (e.g. remembering conform toing an advert, but not particular content). This distinction is important, because when there is divided attention during exposure, there is a significant effect upon recollection, but little or no effect upon familiarity (cf. studies by Craik et al. 1996). Advertising implication Because one is more likely to pay uncomplete attention rather than full attention to advertising, familiarity with advertising is more likely than recollection of specifics from the advertisement.This underscores the importance of maintaining a consistent look and feel over time (Percy et al. 2001), encouraging familiarity, and utilising imagery that will elicit a positive benefit (associated with the shop) even at low or even sub-c ognitive levels of attention. Additionally, too much exposure, especially massed exposure, could lead to lower levels of specific recollection (as we understand from as long ago as Ebbinghaus 1885). dislocated exposures generally result in check memory, a finding demonstrated in Strongs simulations (1974) of various media schedules ground upon Zielskes work, and more tardily in fMRI studies conducted by Wagner et al. (1998). believeing to do something in the future (e.g. buying an advertised brand the next time you are shopping) is described by psychologists asprospective memory. sensation and McDaniel (1990, 1997 with Shaw) have offered a useful way of looking at this idea of prospective memory, distinguishing between what they call event-based prospective memory, where we want to remember to do something at a specific event, and time-based prospective memory, when one wishes to remember to do something at a specific time in the future. An example of event-based prospective m emory would be wanting to buy a new brand the next time you are at the store. An example of time-based prospective memory would be making sure you are home at 3p.m. to meet the spoken communication man. Why people experience prospective memory failure is that they are usually so absent-minded with other things in their lives that when the event occurs, or the time arrives when it is necessary to remember to do something, the correct standstills in memory are not activated.Advertising implication Prospective memory failure may be denigrated in advertising by using distinctive cues that are unlikely to be associated with other long memories (especially for competitive brands). It is important to establish links in memory with the enchant category need in such a way that when a purchase or usage occasion occurs, it will trigger off a memory of the intention to act. This is especially true for recognition-driven brand awareness, which means for most package goods products. In the store point-of-purchase material as well as packaging must be both sufficiently informative to trigger the stored memory of an intention to buy, and be distinctive enough to minimise confusion with other brand memories. Shoppers are almost always in a hurry and preoccupied with other things when they are in a store, and this may get in the way of attending to the earmark prospective memory cue. This is just the sort of thing that goes on when a salesman creates a distraction, hoping you will forget all about your initial good intentions not to be influenced by his pitch, as we know from the literature on pliant behaviour (cf. Cialdini 2001). THE SIN OF BLOCKINGWe are all familiar with the sin of blocking, that all-too-familiar experience of recognising someone but not being able to remember their realise. According to Schacter (2001), blocking is not the same thing as absent-mindedness or transience. In the case of blocking, the memory has been encoded and stored, unlike absent- mindedness. In fact, an detachretrieval cue could be in place, but the association is just not made. Unlike transience, with blocking, the information is still in memory, but remains just out of reach when required. Because blocking generally occurs when trying to remember name, it potentially can be a problem for brand name. Blocking seems to originate in the left temporal pole, where there is a breakdown in the link made between the characteristics associated with something and the name by which it is known.The reason people often have trouble remembering someones name is that a persons name tends to be isolated in memory from any conceptual friendship about that person and, as a result, difficult to retrieve. Most models of name retrieval hold that activation of phonological representations in memory occur only after activation of conceptual and visual representations. This is why it is easier to recall something about a person than to recall their name. It is also what can l ead to remembering something about a product without being able to recall the brand name. Interestingly, names that are most likely to be blocked are familiar ones which have not recently been encountered (Burke et al. 1991. Advertising implication Brand names that are not well integrated or related to obvious associations with category need will be highly susceptible to blocking. If there are no logical and immediate links in memory between a brand name and the category need, there is the risk of occasional blocking. Arbitrary or more abstract brand names will be blocked more often than descriptive brand names, even when those names are equally familiar to people (cf. Brdant Valentine 1998).Brand names such as Vitalegs (a herbal gel that relieves pall legs) and Soft Scrub (a cleanser that enables you to clean without harsh scratching) illustrate good descriptive brand names that are less likely to be susceptible to blocking. To minimise blocking, it is necessary to suppress the r etrieval of recently encountered information that is related to a recall cue so that the mind is not cluttered with irrelevancies that could interfere with the desired memory. Advertising implication When a brand possesses identical or similar benefits as the star(p) brand in its category, it will be that much harder to build an association for those benefits with the brand because of well-educated snag from advertising for the leading brand. This again suggests the need to have copy (and packaging as well as other marketing communication) unique to a brand in sight to avoid multiple connections inmemory that could minimise or override the desired brand-related memory.Certain retrieval inhibitions that lead to blocking can be released if we encounter a sufficiently powerful cue (e.g. nondeclarative emotional memories) that helps us re-experience something in the same way in which it was initially experienced. Appropriate triggers in advertising or other marketing communication t hat elicit the correct emotional memories may help overcome retrieval inhibitions, and release positive memories for a brand. A wonderful advert for Nestls Toll House chocolate chips showing a mother with a pan of chocolate chip cookies fresh out of the oven with a little girl looking on in anticipation perfectly illustrates this point. THE SIN OF MISATTRIBUTIONIf one correctly remembers something learned, but attributes it to the wrong seminal fluid, this is misattribution. Often referred to as unconscioustransference, it causes real problems with eyewitness identification. The problem stems from a strong sense of general familiarity, coupled with an absence of specific recollection. While the consequences of misattribution in advertising are obviously not as serious as they are with eyewitness identification, it can nevertheless cause marketers real problems.Advertising implication Avoiding misattribution requires more than s call for retrieving appropriate benefits from memory. The benefit must be linked together in memory in such a way that you make the correct association of the brand with its benefit claim. This linking process is known as memory binding. All of the important brandbenefit associations in advertising must be bound together by the telephone receiver into a unifying whole at the time of encoding. When advertising for different brands is visually or communicatoryly similar, this memory binding is unlikely to occur, leading to memory conjunction error.Memory conjunction errors occur because people misattribute strong familiarity with similar (even if not identical) things from more than one quotation as coming from a champion source brand advertising in our case. Interestingly, a strong visualverbal congruence can help minimise misattribution (cf. Schacter et al. 1999). A recent series of adverts for Good Humor-Breyers uses the exact format and headline (Less fat, fewer calories, no guilt) for deuce-ace brands Popsicle, Breyers and Klon dike. This would seem to almost encourage misattribution.THE SIN OF SUGGESTIBILITYSuggestibility in memory occurs because one tends to include information that has been learned from an outside source as something personally experienced. This information may come from any external source, including advertising or other marketing communication. While suggestibility is similar to the sin of misattribution, misattribution does not require suggestions from outside sources. just now when the two combine, it is quite possible for us to develop memories of something which in fact never occurred. Advertising implication Interestingly, while suggestibility may be a sin of memory, in the world of marketing communication this sin may often become a blessing. For example, suggestive questions may produce memory distortions by creating source memory problems.As a result, advertising that utilises questions that remind people of a favourable brand association could occasion a memory for that posi tive experience, even if it never occurred, e.g. Remember how easy it is to remove those skanky stains when you use our brand? Schacter has suggested that if you embellish a fake memory with vivid mental images it should make it look and feel like a true memory. This is based upon work done by Hyman and Pentland (1996) in successfully creating false childishness memories via suggestion, simply by asking subjects about things that never occurred. One of the important conclusions they displace from their work is that these false memories produce vivid visual images. Advertising implication The application to advertising is obvious. If a suggested favourable experience with a brand is reinforced with a strong visual image of such an experience, it should help seed a memory of a positive experience. In an extension of these ideas, we know that one of the best ways to elicit early childhood memories is to ask someone to visualise themselves as children.While there is no evidence that anyone can remember anything much earlier than about two years of age, because the areas of the brain needed for episodic memory are not fully mature until that age, with suggestive visual image techniques one can create false memories for events going back almost to birth (cf. Spanos et al. 1999). The fundamental here, as in all suggestibility, is expectancy. If one is instructed to expect something, and it seems plausible, it is possible to create rather strong false memories. Advertising implication It is very difficult tosuggest a false memory for something that runs counter to a recent or strong existing memory. If you dont like a brand, advertising is not likely to create a false memory that you do nor should you try. But if a brand is one of a set of brands apply by the receiver, it is certainly possible to suggest more positive experiences with that brand. And if it is a brand they have not used, if the advertising can relate it to a positive experience from childhood, it is quite possible to suggest positive memories for the benefit, and then link it to the brand. THE SIN OF BIASThe sin of bias reflects how current understandings, beliefs and feelings have the ability to distort how one interprets new experiences and the memory of them. Biases that are associated with memory of past experiences will greatly influence how one perceives and understands new information or situations. Schacter talks about five major types of bias consistency, change, hindsight, egocentric and sterile biases. Gazzaniga (1998) has identified something in the left brain that he calls an interpreter that continuously draws upon peoples experiences and understanding of things in order to provide some stability to their psychological world. This would seem to be the neurological source of biases, and utilises such things as inferences, rationalisations and generalisations in relating the past with the present, enabling people to justify their present attitudes with past exp eriences and feelings. The left brain interpreter, however, is mediated by systems in the right brain that are more attuned to actual representations of what is going on in the world around us. Consistency and change biasConsistency bias reflects a tendency to exonerate (or believe) today in a fashion consistent with how one remembers similar previous experiences. When this happens, current experiences and feelings are filtered through and made to match memories of those past experiences and feelings. Because memories are not exact, people tend to infer their past beliefs, attitudes and feelings from what they are experiencing today. Advertising implication This suggests that for people who hold current positive attitudes toward a brand, advertising could imply they are of long standing. For brand switchers who include a particular brand in their purchase set, advertisingcould imply a long standing preference for that brand You know you have always care this brand, why not buy mor e? Something similar occurs with change bias, where one remembers something being worse than it actually was, making what they feel now an improvement by comparison. Both consistency and change bias can occur because they help reduce cognitive dissonance, even when someone is not really aware of the source of the inconsistency they are trying to manage (Lieberman et al. 2000). Hindsight biasHindsight bias is that familiar feeling that one has always known something would happen after becoming aware of the outcome. One is reconstructing the past to make it consistent with the present. The key here seems to be an activation of general knowledge. The new information is integrated with other general knowledge in semantic memory, and is not distinguished as such in making judgements. There is evidence that this selective recall is a function of the general knowledge that influences perception and comprehension, and a vulnerability to misattribution. Advertising implication Hindsight bias would seem to indicate that when undetermined to advertising or other marketing communication one will recall benefit claims that are not actually made, but which would have been evaluate to be there because of the claims that actually were made. Work by Carli (1999) tends to support this idea. Recent adverts for Infusium 23 set up a beforeafter case, but leave out the before picture, with the headline You really think I would let them publish the before picture? This clever execution encourages hindsight bias as you imagine the before tomentum cerebri problem. Egocentric biasThe self plays an important usance in ones ongoing mental life, and is at the spread-eagle of egocentric bias. When encoding new information by relating it to the self, memory for that information will be better than other types of encoding. This is because people are more likely to value their own understanding of things, among other reasons because the self-concept plays a key role in regulating mental activity. As Taylor (1989) and her colleagues have pointed out, individuals do not see themselves objectively. Advertising implication The implication is obvious include personal references in advertising and other marketing communication. Moreover, givenour tendency to see ourselves in a positive light, it follows that memories related to ourselves will be seen in a self-enhancing light. This suggests that copy asking people to remember a situation in a positive light should encourage an egocentric memory bias, e.g. remember when you . In the same way, egocentric bias can result from exaggerating the difficulty of past experiences remember how hard it was to . This idea is well illustrated in a campaign for National Rails Senior Railcard, where a dated-looking picture of a young child is featured, with headlines like Remember what it was like to go somewhere for the first time and Remember how it felt just to let yourself go. THE SIN OF PERSISTENCEResearch has shown that emotional ly charged experiences are better remembered than less emotional occasions. The sin of persistence involves remembering things you wish you would forget, and it is strongly associated with ones emotional experiences. Advertising implication Emotionally-charged information automatically attracts attention and even in the briefest exposure, the emotional significance of it will be retrieved from nondeclarative emotional memory, and evaluated as to how that information will be encoded. Understanding the emotional associations generated by specific advertising is critical. Because people are more likely to remember the central focus of emotionally arousing information rather than peripheral details, it is essential to tie the brand in marketing communication to the appropriate emotion. Otherwise, it will become peripheral to the information conveyed (a problem with a the great unwashed of highly entertaining advertising). There is evidence that persistence thrives in negative emotional situations such as disappointment, sadness and regret. Ones memory of traumatic experiences is persistent, and while these unwanted memories may occur in any of the senses, visual memories are by far the most common. Research reported by Ochsner (2000) supports this idea.He found that when people recognise a positive visual image they tend to just say it is familiar to them. But when they recognise negative visual images, people relate detailed, specific memories of what they thought and felt when they were originally exposed to the picture. Advertising implication All of this underscores the importance of the visual images in advertising and other forms of marketing communication. Because persistence thrives in a negative emotional climate,if advertising illustrates disappointment or problems dealing with a situation, which is single-minded by using the brand, this should tap into any persistent memories of product dissatisfaction (always assuming such dissatisfaction). It also s uggests that for appropriate product categories (especially those reflecting high-involvement informational decisions such as medical or other insurance, financial planning, and so forth) visual reminders of past problems which could be avoided with a brand should be an effective strategy. Such a strategy should also be equally effective in situations where there is strong psychological risk involved, e.g. reminding young people of a social disaster which would never occur if they used our brand.The root of much of this kind of activity is centred indoors the amygdala, the source of nondeclarative emotional memory. It is the amygdala that regulates memory storage, and can release hormones that can force us to remember an experience vividly (LeDoux 1996). And as we have already noted, this response by the amygdala is much more likely to occur for negative than positive experience. Advertising implication For appropriate product categories, it could make sense to create situations in advertising that suggest possible threats to the receivers wellbeing. This threat may then well lever upon active memory when thinking about the category, with our brand linked to avoiding the trouble. This is well illustrated in a recent advert for Imitrex, an ethical drug for migraine, that uses the headline I cant let a migraine call the shots thats why I use Imitrex. SUMMARYSchacter has provided us with an extremely useful framework for looking at memory problems his seven sins of memory. Each of these imperfections (in his words) has the potential for interfering with the successful processing of advertising and other marketing communication. Recent work in neurobiology, utilising the recent technology of fMRIs (functional magnetic resonance imaging) and PET scans (positron emission tomography), has shown us that our earlier understanding of memories as snapshots stored away in the mind ready to be recalled is not how the brain works. Memories for objects and experiences are decomposed into a number of different parts and those parts are stored in various areas of the brain, time lag to be reassembled and remembered. This underscores why memories are rarelyperfect, and why they can be potentially unreliable. As this sermon makes clear, effective communication faces a number of formidable hurdles in memory. However, forewarned with this knowledge, we are in a better position to avoid or at least minimise some of these potential problems.To help advertising communication overcome the seven sins of memory, advertisers shouldensure the message is carefully integrated with how a brand is understood (transcience) encourage elaboration of points the target is interested in remembering (transcience)use personal references, especially to positive memories (bias)imply current positive brand attitudes are of long standing (bias)tie brands to appropriate emotions (blocking, persistence)use distinctive cues not likely to be associated with other longterm memories ( absent-mindedness) create a unique brandbenefit claim link (misattribution)establish links in memory to appropriate category need (absentmindedness) make sure those links are well integrated with obvious associations to the category need (blocking) ensure a consistent look and feel over time to encourage familiarity (absent-mindedness)use strong visual images to create or reinforce positive memories associated with the brand (suggestibility)utilise reminders of past problems that could be avoided or solved by the brand (persistence). If these points are considered in the creation of advertising executions, one is well on the way to avoiding, or at least minimising, problems inherent in how memory works. REFERENCESBrdant, S. Valentine, T. (1998) Descriptiveness and proper name retrieval. Memory, 6, pp. 199206. Burke, A., Mackay, D.G., Worthley, J.S. E. Wade (1991) On the tip of the mother tongue what causes word failure in young and older adults? Journal of Memory and Language, 30 , pp. 237246. Carli, I.L.L. (1999) Cognitive reconstruction, hindsight, and reactions to victims and perpetrators. Personality and fond Psychology Bulletin, 25, pp. 966979. Cialdini, R. (2001) Influence Science and Practice (4th edn). Boston Allyn and Bacon. Craik, F.I.M., Govoni, R., Naveh-Benjamin, M. Anderson, N.D. (1996) The effects of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory. Journal of data-based Psychology General, 125, pp. 159180. Ebbinghaus, H. (1885/1964) Memory A Contribution to Experimental Psychology. New York Dover. Einstein, G.O. McDaniel, M.A. (1990) Normalaging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, pp. 717726. Einstein, G.O., McDaniel, M.A. Shaw, P. (1997) Aging and prospective memory the influence of increased task demands at encoding and retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 12, pp. 479488. Gazzaniga, M.S. (1998) The split brain revisited. Scientific American, 279, pp. 5055. Grif fiths, R.E. (1997) What Emotions truly Are. Chicago The University of Chicago Press. Hyman, I.E. Jr. Pentland, J. (1996) The role of mental imagery in the creation of false childhood memories. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, pp. 101117. LeDoux, J.E. (1996) The Emotional Brain. New York Simon and Schuster. Lieberman, M.D., Ochsner, K.N., Gilbert, D.T. Schacter, D.L. (2000) Do amnesiacs exhibit cognitive dissonance reduction? The role of explicit memory and attention in attitude change.Psychological Science. Ochsner, K.N. (2000) Are affective events richly recalled or simply familiar? The experience and process of recognizing feelings past. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 124, pp. 242261. Percy, L., Rossiter, J.R. Elliott R. (2001) Strategic Advertising Management. Oxford Oxford University Press. Schacter, D.L. (2001) The vii Sins of Memory How the Mind Forgets and Remembers. Boston Houghton Mifflin Company. Schacter, D.L., Israel, L. Racine, C. (1999) Suppressin g false recognition the distinctiveness heuristic. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, pp. 124. Simons, D.J. Levin, D.T. (1998) Failure to observe changes to people during realworld interactions. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 4, pp. 501506. Spanos, N.P., Burgess, C.A., Burgess, M.F., Samuels, C. Blois, W.O. (1999) Creating false memories of infancy with hypnotic and non-hypnotic procedures. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, pp. 201218. Strong, E.C. (1974) The use of field data-based observations in estimating advertising recall. Journal of Marketing Research, 11, pp. 369378. Taylor, S.E. (1989) Positive Illusions. New York Basic Books. Wagner, A.D., Schacter, D.L., Rolfe, M., Koutstaal, W., Maril, A., Dale, A.M., Rosen, B.R. Buckner, R.L. (1998) Building memories remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science, 281, pp. 11881191.

No comments:

Post a Comment